archived

Final Recommendation Statement

Elevated Lead Levels in Childhood and Pregnancy: Screening, 1996

January 01, 1996

Recommendations made by the USPSTF are independent of the U.S. government. They should not be construed as an official position of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality or the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

This Recommendation is out of date

It has been replaced by the following: Elevated Blood Lead Levels in Children and Pregnant Women: Screening (2019)

Recommendation Summary

Population Recommendation Grade
Children age 12 months or younger Screening for elevated lead levels by measuring blood lead at least once at age 12 months is recommended for all children at increased risk of lead exposure. B
General population There is insufficient evidence to recommend for or against trying to prevent lead exposure by counseling families to control lead dust by repeated household cleaning, or to optimize caloric, iron, and calcium intake specifically to reduce lead absorption. C
Asymptomatic pregnant women There is insufficient evidence to recommend for or against routine screening for lead exposure in asymptomatic pregnant women. C

Recommendation Information

Full Recommendation:

Recommendations made by the USPSTF are independent of the U.S. government. They should not be construed as an official position of the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality or the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

Expand All

Prevalence

The prevalence of elevated blood lead levels in the U.S. population has declined 78% in the past decade,1 due primarily to marked declines in lead in gasoline, soldered cans, and air.1-6 In a 1988-1991 national survey of children aged 1-5 years, 9% and 0.5% had blood lead levels ≥10 micro-g/dL and ≥25 micro-g/dL, respectively, down from 88% and 9% a decade before.7 (The units micro-g/dL will be used throughout this chapter to convert to micro-mol/L, divide by 20.72.) Prevalence varies widely among different communities and populations, however, with studies reporting 2-41% of children having blood lead levels ≥10 micro-g/dL, 0-3% ≥25 micro-g/dL, and 0-0.5% ≥40 micro-g/dL.8-19 Current national data for pregnant women have not been published, but only 0.5% of U.S. women aged 12-49 years of age have blood lead levels ≥10 micro-g/dL.7 Two large surveys of low-income pregnant women found 0%20 and 6%21 with blood lead levels >15 micro-g/dL.

Risk Factors for Elevated Lead Levels

The highest mean blood lead levels in the U.S. occur in children aged 1-2 years (mean 4.1 micro-g/dL) and in adults ≥50 years of age (4.0 micro-g/dL), with the lowest in adolescents (1.6 micro-g/dL).7 Among adults, geometric mean levels are significantly higher in males than in females. Correlates of higher blood lead levels at all ages include minority race/ethnicity, central city residence, low income, low educational attainment, and residence in the Northeast region of the U.S.7,22 These factors are associated with increased exposure to important lead sources, including dilapidated, pre-1950 housing with lead-based paint, lead-soldered pipes and household lead dust and lead in dust and soil from heavy traffic and industry.22-27 Other potential sources of household lead exposure include clothing or waste material brought home by workers in lead-based industries or hobbies, lead-based paint and dust contamination in pre-1950 housing that is undergoing remodeling or renovation, dietary intake from lead-soldered cans and lead-based pottery, and traditional ethnic remedies.23,24,28

Neurotoxic Effects of Lead Exposure in Children

Very high levels of inorganic lead exposure can produce serious neurologic complications, which may result in death or long-term sequelae.23 A growing number of studies have reported associations between neurotoxic effects and blood lead levels once thought to be harmless. Adequately designed and conducted prospective cohort studies from a broad range of child populations have reported that a rise in blood lead from 10 to 20 micro-g/dL is associated with a likely decrement of about 2 points (reported range -6 to +1) in intelligence test scores (IQ).29-35 In these studies, the mean blood lead levels at age 1-2 years (7.7-35.4 micro-g/dL) were higher than the current U.S. mean for this age group (4 micro-g/dL), but most levels were below 35 micro-g/dL. A meta-analysis36 that included the five oldest of these cohort studies concluded that a doubling of blood lead levels from 10 to 20 micro-g/dL measured at age 2 years was associated with a statistically significant mean reduction of 1-2 IQ points evidence was inconclusive regarding an association of IQ with mean postnatal blood lead levels. Although most cross-sectional studies evaluating the association of tooth and blood lead with IQ suffer from methodologic problems such as selection bias and limited adjustment for covariates, they have been generally consistent in reporting small negative effects of elevated lead levels on IQ. [e.g., in36, 37] A meta-analysis that included studies of whole tooth lead published since 1979 reported a statistically significant 1 point reduction in IQ associated with a doubling of tooth lead from 5 to 10 micro-g/g.36 Evidence is not sufficient to quantify the exact nature of the relationship between IQ and higher blood lead levels (i.e., 40-100 micro-g/dL). Cross-sectional studies38-42 have consistently reported small, inverse associations between blood or tooth lead and reaction (attentional) performance, but studies evaluating the effect of mildly elevated lead levels on other measures of neurodevelopmental function (e.g., behavior, learning disorders, auditory function) have produced inconclusive results. These have been less thoroughly evaluated than IQ, however.

In most studies, the size of the estimates of lead effects on IQ are reduced when adjusted for potentially confounding variables,36 suggesting that some of the observed association may be due to imperfectly measured or unmeasured covariates. Studies in rodents and primates, however, which can avoid most of the methodologic weaknesses of observational studies in humans, report cognitive, attentional, and behavioral deficits, as well as auditory and visual dysfunction, with mildly elevated blood lead levels,43-45 supporting a causal relationship between low-level lead exposure and neurotoxic effects in children. Studies demonstrating laboratory abnormalities (e.g., impaired vitamin D metabolism) in persons with blood lead levels as low as 10-15 micro-g/dL23,46-48 also support a causal relationship.

Adverse Effects of Lead Exposure on Pregnancy Outcomes

The effects of very high blood lead levels during pregnancy on reproductive outcomes such as abortion and stillbirth have been recognized for many years.23 Observational studies in pregnant women with blood lead levels <30 micro-g/dL have reported associations between elevated levels and birth weight, length of gestation (including preterm delivery), and neonatal head circumference.49-56 The associations have been small, variable in direction of effect, and not statistically significant in most studies. These studies failed to detect important effects on other reproductive outcomes. Inconsistent results may be due in part to imprecise measures of fetal lead exposure.55-59 All but one 34 of six previously cited cohort studies,29-34 as well as the meta-analysis described above,36 reported no association between antenatal or perinatal maternal blood lead levels and full-scale IQ measured at preschool or school age. Although very high lead levels in pregnancy are clearly hazardous, the adverse effects on the fetus of antepartum lead levels in the range typically found in the U.S. are not established.

Other Adverse Effects of Lead Exposure

Lead exposure affects many organ systems, including cardiovascular, renal, and hepatic, but most clinically apparent (i.e., symptomatic) effects occur with blood lead levels ≥50 micro-g/dL.23,60-63 Small increases in systolic blood pressure have been asso-ciated with mildly elevated blood lead levels (i.e., 1-3 mm Hg for a rise in blood lead from 10 to 20 micro-g/dL) in most large, population-based, cross-sectional studies evaluating nonpregnant adults and pregnant women.64-70 In children, evidence of blood pressure effects is more limited: one crosssectional study found no association between elevated blood lead levels (range 7-70 micro-g/dL) and elevated blood pressure.71 Adverse effects on height from lead levels well below 40 micro-g/dL have been suggested by analyses of national cross-sectional data,72,73 but cohort studies with more extensive covariate adjustment report either transient or no effect of elevated lead levels (peak sample means 11-17 micro-g/dL) on growth.35,74,75

Accuracy of Screening Tests

Screening tests considered for detecting lead exposure include blood lead and free erythrocyte (or zinc) protoporphyrin levels. Blood lead concentration is the more sensitive of the two for detecting modest lead exposure, but its accuracy, precision and reliability can be affected by environmental lead contamination during blood collection, day-to-day biologic variability, and laboratory analytic variation. Lead contamination of collecting equipment, and skin contamination during capillary sampling, may each positively bias blood lead levels by up to 1.0 micro-g/dL, on average, although individual effects of skin contamination may be much greater.76-80 Studies defining abnormal results as blood lead levels above 10 or 20 micro-g/dL have reported false-positive rates of 3-9% for capillary sampling compared to simultaneously collected venous blood lead.77-78 Day-to-day biologic variability and trends over time contribute to higher false-positive rates for initial capillary samples when compared to results from venous testing done at a later date.77,81 False-negative rates with capillary sampling appear to be lower, reported in one study as 1-8% compared to venous blood.78 In published surveys,76,82 80-90% of clinical laboratories participating in proficiency testing programs met performance criteria for blood lead (within ± micro-g/dL of target values, for values <40 micro-g/dL82) unpublished national data show >95% of participating laboratories meeting these criteria and >80% achieving accuracy to within ±2 micro-g/dL of target values (unpublished data, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, November 1993). Nonparticipating laboratories are likely to be less proficient. Reported blood lead values may differ by as much as 5 micro-g/dL from true values due to these sources of variability and bias, which may affect the predictive value of a positive test. Results from capillary samples may vary even more, although recent studies suggest the positive bias can be reduced with increased attention to reducing skin lead contamination.77,78

The erythrocyte protoporphyrin (EP) test, an indirect measure of lead exposure based on lead's effects on the hematopoietic system, is unaffected by contamination with environmental lead and is easily performed on capillary blood specimens, making it more acceptable for use with young patients. Erythrocyte (or zinc) protoporphyrin is insensitive, however, to modest elevations in blood lead levels.21,83-89 The test also lacks specificity,21,83,84,86,87,90 thus limiting its predictive value. In one study, EP measurements were taken on 47,230 suburban and rural children although 4.7% of the children had an elevated erythrocyte protoporphyrin level, only 0.6% had elevated blood lead levels.91

In communities where there is a low prevalence of lead levels requiring individual intervention with chelation or residential lead hazard control, blood lead screening will have a low yield, and many unaffected children will be tested at potentially high cost and inconvenience. A questionnaire that can predict those at high risk for elevated lead levels would allow targeted screening in low prevalence areas, increasing the yield of blood testing by increasing the pretest probability of elevated lead levels in those who are tested. Cross-sectional studies13-15,92-93 93a in urban and suburban, mostly midwestern, populations have shown that one or more positive responses to five questions (about exposures to deteriorated paint from older or renovated housing, to other lead-poisoned children, or to lead-related hobbies or industry)128 detects 64-87% of children with blood lead levels ≥10 micro-g/dL. Three studies reported higher sensitivities (81-100%) for blood lead levels ≥15-20 micro-g/dL.15,92 93a None of these studies evaluated the ability of questionnaires to detect levels above 20 micro-g/dL, in part because so few patients had levels so high. Specificity among the studies ranged from 32% to 75%. In the samples with a lower prevalence (2-7%) of levels ≥10 micro-g/dL, the proportion of those with a negative questionnaire who had elevated blood lead levels was predictably low (0.2-3.5%), but increased to 19% when the population prevalence of elevated lead levels was higher (17-28%). 

Return to Table of Contents

Screening tests considered for detecting lead exposure include blood lead and free erythrocyte (or zinc) protoporphyrin levels. Blood lead concentration is the more sensitive of the two for detecting modest lead exposure, but its accuracy, precision and reliability can be affected by environmental lead contamination during blood collection, day-to-day biologic variability, and laboratory analytic variation. Lead contamination of collecting equipment, and skin contamination during capillary sampling, may each positively bias blood lead levels by up to 1.0 micro-g/dL, on average, although individual effects of skin contamination may be much greater.76-80 Studies defining abnormal results as blood lead levels above 10 or 20 micro-g/dL have reported false-positive rates of 3-9% for capillary sampling compared to simultaneously collected venous blood lead.77-78 Day-to-day biologic variability and trends over time contribute to higher false-positive rates for initial capillary samples when compared to results from venous testing done at a later date.77,81 False-negative rates with capillary sampling appear to be lower, reported in one study as 1-8% compared to venous blood.78 In published surveys,76,82 80-90% of clinical laboratories participating in proficiency testing programs met performance criteria for blood lead (within ±4 micro-g/dL of target values, for values <40 micro-g/dL82) unpublished national data show >95% of participating laboratories meeting these criteria and >80% achieving accuracy to within ±2 micro-g/dL of target values (unpublished data, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, November 1993). Nonparticipating laboratories are likely to be less proficient. Reported blood lead values may differ by as much as 5 micro-g/dL from true values due to these sources of variability and bias, which may affect the predictive value of a positive test. Results from capillary samples may vary even more, although recent studies suggest the positive bias can be reduced with increased attention to reducing skin lead contamination.77,78

The erythrocyte protoporphyrin (EP) test, an indirect measure of lead exposure based on lead's effects on the hematopoietic system, is unaffected by contamination with environmental lead and is easily performed on capillary blood specimens, making it more acceptable for use with young patients. Erythrocyte (or zinc) protoporphyrin is insensitive, however, to modest elevations in blood lead levels.21,83-89 The test also lacks specificity,21,83,84,86,87,90 thus limiting its predictive value. In one study, EP measurements were taken on 47,230 suburban and rural children although 4.7% of the children had an elevated erythrocyte protoporphyrin level, only 0.6% had elevated blood lead levels.91

In communities where there is a low prevalence of lead levels requiring individual intervention with chelation or residential lead hazard control, blood lead screening will have a low yield, and many unaffected children will be tested at potentially high cost and inconvenience. A questionnaire that can predict those at high risk for elevated lead levels would allow targeted screening in low prevalence areas, increasing the yield of blood testing by increasing the pretest probability of elevated lead levels in those who are tested. Cross-sectional studies[[13-15,92-93a]] in urban and suburban, mostly midwestern, populations have shown that one or more positive responses to five questions (about exposures to deteriorated paint from older or renovated housing, to other lead-poisoned children, or to lead-related hobbies or industry)128 detects 64-87% of children with blood lead levels ≥10 micro-g/dL. Three studies reported higher sensitivities (81-100%) for blood lead levels ≥15-20 micro-g/dL.[[15,92,93a]] None of these studies evaluated the ability of questionnaires to detect levels above 20 micro-g/dL, in part because so few patients had levels so high. Specificity among the studies ranged from 32% to 75%. In the samples with a lower prevalence (2-7%) of levels ≥10 micro-g/dL, the proportion of those with a negative questionnaire who had elevated blood lead levels was predictably low (0.2-3.5%), but increased to 19% when the population prevalence of elevated lead levels was higher (17-28%

Return to Table of Contents

Detection of lead exposure before the development of potentially irreversible complications permits the clinician to recommend environmental interventions to limit further exposure and, when necessary, to begin medical treatment with chelating agents. Early detection may also result in interventions that prevent exposure of other children to lead (the child with elevated blood lead level acting as a sentinel for a hazardous environment). There is relatively little convincing evidence that these interventions improve health, however. One issue is that most available studies in asymptomatic children evaluate the effects of various interventions on blood lead levels rather than on clinical outcomes. Second, blood lead levels typically decline with the passage of time. On average, blood lead levels in childhood decrease with age after peaking at about 2 years of age, even without intervention.7 Longitudinal studies of asymptomatic children with elevated lead levels have shown reductions in blood lead levels after short- and long-term follow-up in the absence of any intervention,94,95 a result attributable at least in part to regression to the mean, random variation, and laboratory error. To evaluate adequately the effects of interventions on blood lead levels, studies must take into account these changes over time, preferably by the use of controls who do not receive the intervention.

Effect of Screening on Clinical Outcomes

Evidence is not available to demonstrate that universal screening for blood lead results in better clinical outcomes than either screening targeted to high-risk persons or individualized testing in response to clinical suspicion. Several older studies reported that, compared to historical results from individualized testing, intensive screening programs targeted to children in high-risk neighborhoods reduced case fatality rates, mortality rates, and proportions of children detected with very high blood lead levels or who developed symptomatic lead poisoning.96-98 In the absence of concurrent controls, it is not clear whether the reported reductions in mortality and case fatality rates were due to screening, or to improvements in medical care over time. Reductions in mean lead levels may also have been due to secular trends, changes in screening tests, and to screening greater numbers of children, including many at low risk for severe lead poisoning. Thus, the available evidence regarding the efficacy of screening programs is weak.

Effect of Interventions to Lower Blood Lead Levels on Clinical Outcomes

In contrast to substantial evidence that chelating agents benefit children with symptomatic lead poisoning, few studies have compared potential clinical benefits of chelation therapy with its adverse effects in asymptomatic children. Ethical considerations preclude such trials for children with blood lead levels above 45 micro-g/dL. A large randomized controlled trial assessing the effect of chelation therapy on IQ in young children with venous blood lead concentrations of 20-45 micro-g/dL is currently under way (G. Rhoads, personal communication, Environmental and Occupational Health Sciences Institute, Piscataway, NJ, January 1994). An observational study99,100 compared children with blood lead levels between 13 and 46 micro-g/dL (median 30 micro-g/dL), who did and did not receive EDTA chelation therapy depending on the results of a lead mobilization test. There was no effect of chelation on IQ at either 7 weeks or 6 months follow-up after controlling for age and initial IQ. Changes in concentrations of blood lead, bone lead, and EP also did not differ significantly between chelated and unchelated children. The greatest reductions in blood lead were associated with the highest initial lead levels, independent of chelation. The method of treatment assignment (i.e., based on a positive mobilization test) was most likely to have biased the study toward finding an effect of chelation, yet no effect was observed. There is thus little evidence presently available to confirm a clinical benefit from chelation therapy for children with lead levels <45 micro-g/dL. A comprehensive literature review found no studies evaluating clinical effects of residential lead hazard control. Their effects on blood lead levels are reviewed below.

Effects of Chelation Therapy on Blood Lead Levels

In uncontrolled experiments and case series in asymptomatic children with initial blood lead levels ranging from 40 to 471 micro-g/dL, chelating agents reduced blood lead levels substantially, to levels <40-70 micro-g/dL (varying with initial levels) these reductions were maintained for weeks to years after therapy was discontinued.101-105 Most of these children were also returned to homes that had undergone lead hazard reduction, and the effect of this additional intervention was not specifically evaluated. Chelating agents have caused short-term reductions in blood lead levels in children whose pretreatment values ranged from 20 to 49 micro-g/dL in nonrandomized comparative trials, cohort studies, and uncontrolled experiments these reductions have not been sustained over longer periods in the absence of repeated or continuing chelation therapy or environmental interventions.104,106-109 Most of these studies did not report whether chelation therapy was combined with environmental interventions. With such weak evidence, including the previously cited cohort study reporting no effects of chelation on IQ,99 it is difficult to make a convincing argument that chelation therapy to lower moderately elevated blood lead levels has a long-term benefit.

Effect of Residential Lead Hazard Control on Blood Lead Levels

For most asymptomatic children with elevated lead levels, the primary goal of intervention is to reduce exposure to lead-contaminated paint, dust, and soil in the child's home environment, since these sources account for most excessive lead exposure. Residential lead-based paint hazard control methods have become increasingly effective for reducing exposure to lead paint and lead-contaminated dust.25,110,111 These new techniques are now replacing the older strategies, which often created lead dust during the intervention process, but there are currently few published studies of their effect on blood lead levels.

Because most published studies used older, less effective techniques, the effects of residential interventions on blood lead reported in the literature, and outlined below, probably indicate the minimum possible benefit of residential lead-paint and lead-contaminated dust hazard control. In an early cohort study112 of 184 children with initial blood lead levels ≥50 micro-g/dL, children discharged after chelation therapy to lead-free (i.e., new or completely gutted and renovated) housing had significantly lower mean blood lead levels when compared to children exposed to "legally abated" or to inadequately abated housing (28.8 micro-g/dL vs. 38.5 and 57 micro-g/dL, respectively). Children in lead-free housing also had fewer recurrences of levels ≥50 micro-g/dL at 12 and 24-30 months follow-up. A nonrandomized trial113 of households with children having initial blood lead levels >29 micro-g/dL compared more intensive experimental lead-reduction procedures with the lead-reduction procedures commonly in use in the study community. Neither intervention had any effect on mean dust or blood lead levels as tested 6 months after abatement, but an untreated control group was not included. Published114 and unpublished115 retrospective cohort studies suggest that residential lead paint hazard control is associated with modest declines (4-10 micro-g/dL) in mean blood lead levels in children with initial blood lead levels ≥ 25 micro-g/dL, although in one study114 those with initial blood lead levels <35 micro-g/dL benefitted little from intervention. Case series and uncontrolled experiments, both weak study designs, have also evaluated lead-paint hazard control efforts in children with initial blood lead levels of 25-55 micro-g/dL100,116-119 several were published only as abstracts or summaries.115,120 These studies reported statistically significant declines in mean blood lead levels, ranging from 2.5 to 10.2 micro-g/dL, 6-12 months after residential lead-based paint hazard control. All of the studies cited suffer from important design flaws, such as substantial drop-out rates and inadequate control for confounding variables such as season and age. Despite their flaws, the consistency of the results from these studies suggests a small, beneficial effect of lead-based paint hazard control on blood lead levels. As noted, there are as yet no published studies evaluating the effects on blood lead levels of newer residential lead hazard control techniques.

There are important problems with using one-time residential lead-paint hazard control as the sole method to reduce lead exposure in children.121 Poor, inner-city families tend to move frequently, so that treating the current residence may have limited long-term benefit to the child, although benefit may accrue to other children moving in to that residence (see below). Residential lead-paint hazard control is costly and labor-intensive, resulting in low rates of intervention, especially in poor communities.24,122 Lead dust is ubiquitous and highly mobile, so that recontamination by nearby lead sources, including soil lead, may occur after lead-paint hazard control efforts take place in a dwelling.110,123,124 These problems indicate a need for additional individual interventions, as well as more comprehensive community-based interventions, to reduce household lead exposure.

The small effect noted in studies evaluating lead-paint hazard control methods may be attributable in part to recontamination of the dwelling by nearby lead sources and from subsequent deterioration of painted surfaces.110,123,124 Several studies have evaluated measures designed to reduce ongoing lead-dust contamination from lead-contaminated paint and soil. In a nonrandomized controlled trial among children with blood lead levels of 30-49 micro-g/dL, having a research team wet-mop all lead-contaminated interior surfaces twice a month with a high-phosphate detergent cleanser resulted in significantly greater adjusted declines in mean blood lead levels of children in intervention households compared to children in control households (6.9 vs. 0.7 micro-g/dL) at 1-year follow-up.125 There have been no controlled studies to evaluate whether counseling families to perform similar cleaning would be equally effective in reducing blood lead levels. In one uncontrolled experiment, the families of 78 children with blood lead levels of 10-35 micro-g/dL, who were living in the vicinity of a defunct lead smelter, received intensive (30-45 minutes) in-home education and literature on prevention of lead exposure.126 The mean blood lead levels in the 51 (65%) children who had follow-up blood lead levels at 4 months declined from 15.0 to 7.8 micro-g/dL (and maximum levels from 35.0 to 12.7 micro-g/dL). Without concurrent controls, it is not possible to determine how much regression to the mean and seasonal and age variations contributed to these reductions in blood lead levels. There is also evidence that clinician counseling at the worksite to reduce lead dust ingestion by workers (e.g., through personal hygiene practices) can significantly reduce mean blood lead levels at 1-year follow-up,127 but this study also lacked controls and may not be generalizable to the residential setting.

A third focus of residential lead hazard control is exposure to soil lead. In a randomized controlled trial123 of young children with initial blood lead levels of 7-24 micro-g/dL, extensive soil abatement, one-time dust abatement, and removal of loose interior paint resulted in a statistically significant reduction in mean blood lead levels of 1.2-1.3 micro-g/dL compared to loose paint removal alone. This clinically insignificant decline was associated with a substantial reduction in soil lead from a median 2,000 to 105 ppm. Preliminary results of the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency's Three City Urban Soil Lead Abatement Demonstration Project similarly suggest that substantial declines in soil lead cause only modest reductions in mildly elevated blood lead concentrations.124 The small effect was due at least in part to rapid recontamination with dust lead in households undergoing soil abatement. Among children living near a closed lead smelter, only 3% of the variance in blood lead levels was attributable to soil lead.127a

An important potential benefit of residential lead hazard control is its effect on the lead levels or clinical outcomes of other children who live in the same household as a child identified with elevated lead levels, or who subsequently move into the remediated residence. The literature review revealed no published evidence evaluating the effect of residential lead hazard control measures on such children. Based on the biokinetics of lead,23 it is reasonable to believe that environmental interventions conducted before children are exposed are likely to prevent increases in blood lead levels more effectively than the same interventions in children who have already been exposed.

Effect of Nutritional Interventions on Blood Lead Levels

In most settings, neither residential lead-based paint or dust hazard control nor chelation therapy is routinely offered to children with blood lead levels <20 micro-g/dL, but some experts have recommended offering these children dietary counseling to reduce their blood lead levels.128 Diets deficient in calories, calcium, and zinc have been associated with increased gastrointestinal absorption of lead,129,130 but there is only limited evidence that counseling to correct such nutritional inadequacies will reduce blood lead levels or prevent further increases. Results of experimental studies of the effects of iron deficiency on lead absorption and retention in adult humans have been equivocal.129,131 In a cohort study of children with initial blood lead levels of 13-46 micro-g/dL,99 all children who were iron deficient or depleted were prescribed iron supplementation. Although most children were still iron deficient at the end of the study, there were improvements in ferritin level that were not associated with either declines in blood lead or improvements in cognitive function. Cross-sectional and cohort studies have failed to establish a clear association between mean blood lead levels and measures of iron status in women at midpregnancy or delivery, in newborns (cord blood), or in children.57,99,131-134

Adverse Effects of Screening and Intervention

The most common adverse effects of screening for elevated lead levels are false-positive fingerstick results, and the anxiety, inconvenience, work or school absenteeism, and financial costs associated with return visits and repeat tests. An EDTA lead mobilization test, used for some children with blood lead levels of 30-44 micro-g/dL,135 requires intramuscular or intravenous infusion, a stay at the clinical center for at least 8 hours, and for young children, application of urine collection bags.136 Residential lead-based paint and dust hazard control, when improperly done,25 may produce acute increases in blood lead levels in resident children and abatement workers, occasionally necessitating hospitalization and chelation therapy.113,116,137-139 Currently recommended techniques for lead hazard reduction are likely to reduce these adverse effects.25 Chelating agents for asymptomatic lead poisoning have also been associated with important adverse effects. EDTA and dimercaprol (BAL) have transient renal, hepatic, and other toxicity, require intravenous or intramuscular injection, and generally require hospitalization for administration.128,140,141 Common adverse effects of d-penicillamine are penicillin-like sensitivity reactions and transient nephrotoxicity there are rare life-threatening reactions.96,105,107,128 Succimer (meso-2,3-dimercaptosuccinic acid, or DMSA) causes mild gastrointestinal and systemic symptoms, rashes, and transient elevations in liver function tests, in up to 10% of cases.104,106,108,142 

Return to Table of Contents

Several states mandate either universal screening for lead exposure or selective screening of populations at high risk for lead exposure.143 Periodic screening of children with blood lead measurement is also required for Medicaid's Early and Periodic Screening, Diagnostic, and Treatment Program.144 The American Academy of Pediatrics145 and the Bright Futures guidelines146 recommend: (a) screen all children for lead exposure at about 12 months of age, and possibly again at about 24 months of age (b)take a history of lead exposure (using questionnaires provided with the guidelines) between the ages of 6 months and 6 years to identify high-risk children who should be screened earlier or more frequently and (c)provide education to parents on safe environmental, occupational, nutritional and hygiene practices to protect their children from lead exposure. Follow-up screening intervals should be based on risk assessment and previous blood lead levels. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) recommends screening all children at 12 months of age using a blood lead test, except in communities where no childhood lead poisoning problem exists high-risk children require earlier and more frequent screening.128 The American Academy of Family Physicians (AAFP)147 and the Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination148 recommend screening all children who are at high risk of lead exposure (e.g., due to exposure to heavy traffic and industry, or to dilapidated older housing). The recommendations of the AAFP are currently under review. The American Medical Association recommends regularly screening all children under the age of 6 years for lead exposure through history-taking and, when appropriate, blood lead testing.149 They recommend that the decision to employ universal or targeted screening be made based on prevalence studies of blood lead levels in the local pediatric population.

 

No major organizations currently recommend screening pregnant women for elevated lead levels. 

Return to Table of Contents

There is fair evidence that screening for elevated lead levels in asymptomatic children at increased risk for lead exposure will improve clinical outcomes. Because there have been no controlled trials directly evaluating screening for elevated lead levels, this conclusion is based on a chain of evidence constructed from studies of weaker design. First, in young asymptomatic children, blood lead levels as low as 10 micro-g/dL are associated with measurable neurodevelopmental dysfunction. Second, although the national prevalence of elevated lead levels has declined substantially in the past decade, a high prevalence persists in some communities, particularly poor urban communities in the northeastern U.S. Third, measurement of venous blood lead concentration is a convenient, reliable, precise and reasonably valid screening test for assessing lead exposure. Fourth, current interventions, including residential lead hazard control and chelation therapy, can reduce blood lead levels in children identified with levels ≥25 micro-g/dL, although the quality of evidence supporting their effectiveness is weak and a beneficial effect on IQ or other clinical outcomes has not yet been demonstrated. There is also weak evidence that screening high-risk children for elevated lead levels results in improved clinical outcome compared to historical controls identified by case-finding. Based on this evidence of the current burden of suffering and the effectiveness of early detection, the Task Force recommends screening children at increased risk for lead exposure.

While no studies have evaluated a specific age at which to screen, the natural history of blood lead levels in children, which increase most rapidly between 6 and 12 months and peak at age 18-24 months, suggests that screening at about 12 months of age is likely to be most effective for the early detection of elevated lead levels.

For those children who are screened and found to have initial blood lead levels <25 micro-g/dL, there is as yet little evidence regarding the effectiveness of early detection and intervention, or of repeated screening to detect further increases in blood lead. Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies suggest that in children ≥2 years, most such levels will decline naturally with time, but elevated levels may persist in children who are chronically exposed.101

There is no direct evidence comparing the outcomes of universal screening with the outcomes from targeted screening for elevated lead levels. Recent studies indicate that the prevalence of elevated blood levels in the U.S. has declined dramatically in the past decade, but that local prevalence is highly variable, with more than 10-fold differences between communities. In a community with a low prevalence of elevated blood lead levels, universal screening may result in disproportionate risks and costs relative to benefits. The prevalence level at which targeted screening can replace universal screening is a public health policy decision requiring consideration of factors in addition to the scientific evidence for effectiveness of early detection, such as available resources, competing public health needs, and costs and availability of alternative approaches to reducing lead exposure. Good quality analyses are needed to determine the population prevalence below which universal lead screening is not cost-effective. Clinicians can consult with their local or state health department regarding appropriate screening policy for the local child population.

In communities where data suggest that universal screening is not indicated, there may nevertheless be some children who are at increased risk of blood lead levels in the range for which individual intervention by chelation therapy or residential lead hazard control has been demonstrated to be effective. These children may have had exposure to lead sources such as lead-based hobbies or industries, traditional ethnic remedies, or lead-based pottery. Selective blood lead screening of such high-risk children is appropriate even in low prevalence communities. There is fair evidence that a validated questionnaire of known and acceptable sensitivity and specificity can identify those at high risk. In several studies, the CDC128 and similar questionnaires correctly identified 64% to 87% of urban and suburban children who had blood lead levels ≥10 micro-g/dL. These questionnaires have not been adequately evaluated as a screening tool to detect higher blood lead levels (e.g., ≥20-25 micro-g/dL), or to detect exposure in other populations (e.g., migrant workers, rural communities). Locale-specific questionnaires that inquire about likely local sources of lead exposure may lead to improved prediction.

As is the case in children, there are no controlled trials evaluating screening for elevated lead levels in pregnant women, nor are there sufficient data to construct an adequate chain of evidence demonstrating benefit. The prevalence of levels >15 micro-g/dL appears to be quite low in pregnant women. There is fair evidence that mildly elevated lead levels during pregnancy are associated with small increases in antepartum blood pressure, but limited evidence that these levels have important adverse effects on reproductive or other outcomes, including intelligence of offspring. An extensive literature search failed to identify studies evaluating screening or intervention for lead exposure in pregnant women. There are potentially important adverse effects of chelation therapy on the fetus, and of residential lead hazard control on both the pregnant woman and fetus if they are not performed according to established standards. Removal to a lead-free environment would theoretically be effective in reducing lead exposure but has not been specifically evaluated in pregnancy. There is thus insufficient evidence to recommend for or against screening pregnant women for the detection of elevated lead levels.

Population-based interventions for the primary prevention of lead exposure are likely to be more effective, and may be more cost-effective, than office-based screening, treatment and counseling. Community, regional, and national environmental lead hazard reduction efforts, such as reducing lead in industrial emissions, gasoline, and cans, have proven highly effective in reducing population blood lead levels.1-6,150,151 Remaining important sources of lead (e.g., lead paint and pipes in older homes, lead-contaminated soil) are, however, more difficult to address on a population-wide basis. Studies of community-based efforts to reduce lead exposure from these and other sources in order to prevent the occurrence of elevated lead levels are ongoing.25,110,152 Evaluation of the effectiveness of community-based interventions, and recommendations regarding their use, are beyond the scope of this document. 

Return to Table of Contents

Screening for elevated lead levels by measuring blood lead at least once at age 12 months is recommended for all children at increased risk of lead exposure ("B" recommendation). All children with identifiable risk factors should be screened, as should children living in communities in which the prevalence of blood lead levels requiring individual intervention, including chelation therapy or residential lead hazard control, is high or is undefined. If capillary blood is used, elevated lead levels should be confirmed by measurement of venous blood lead. The optimal frequency of screening for lead exposure in children, or for repeated testing of children previously found to have elevated blood lead levels, is unknown and is left to clinical discretion consideration should be given to the degree of elevation, the interventions provided, and the natural history of lead exposure, including the typical peak in lead levels at 18-24 months of age.

In communities where the prevalence of blood lead levels requiring individual intervention is low, a strategy of targeted screening, possibly using locale-specific questionnaires of known and acceptable sensitivity and specificity, can be used to identify high-risk children who should have blood lead testing. Examples of individual risk factors include: (a)living in or frequently visiting an older home (built before 1950) with dilapidated paint or with recent or ongoing renovation or remodeling, (b)having close contact with a person who has an elevated lead level, (c)living near lead industry or heavy traffic, (d)living with someone whose job or hobby involves lead exposure, (e)using lead-based pottery, or (f)taking traditional ethnic remedies that contain lead.128 There is currently insufficient evidence to recommend an exact population prevalence below which targeted screening can be substituted for universal screening. The results of cost-benefit analyses, available resources and public health priorities are among the determinants of the prevalence below which targeted screening is recommended for a community. Clinicians can seek guidance from their local or state health department.

There is insufficient evidence to recommend for or against routine screening for lead exposure in asymptomatic pregnant women ("C" recommendation). Recommendations against such screening may be made on the grounds of limited and conflicting evidence regarding the current burden of suffering, high costs, and the potential for adverse effects from intervention.

There is insufficient evidence to recommend for or against trying to prevent lead exposure by counseling families to control lead dust by repeated household cleaning, or to optimize caloric, iron, and calcium intake specifically to reduce lead absorption ("C" recommendation). For high-risk individuals or those living in high-prevalence communities, such recommendations may be made on other grounds, including minimal risk of adverse effects from the cleaning or the dietary advice, and the additional, unrelated benefits from optimizing nutrition (see Chapter 22, Screening for Iron Deficiency Anemia, and Chapter 56, Counseling to Promote a Healthy Diet).

Recommendations regarding community- or population-based interventions for the primary prevention of lead poisoning, assessment of community lead contamination, or the setting of community priorities for lead hazard reduction, are beyond the scope of this document.

Return to Table of Contents

The draft update of this chapter was prepared for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force by Carolyn DiGuiseppi, MD, MPH. 

Return to Table of Contents

1. Pirkle JL, Brody DJ, Gunter EW, et al. The decline in blood lead levels in the United States. The National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys (NHANES). JAMA. 1994;272:284–291. [PubMed]
2. Annest JL, Pirkle JL, Makuc D, et al. Chronological trend in blood lead levels between 1976 and 1980. N Engl J Med. 1983;308:1373–1377. [PubMed]
3. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Blood lead levels—United States, 1988-1991. MMWR. 1994;43:545–548. [PubMed]
4. Hayes EB, McElvaine MD, Orbach HG, et al. Long-term trends in blood lead levels among children in Chicago: relationship to air lead levels. Pediatrics. 1994;93:195–200. [PubMed]
5. Davis JM, Elias RW, Grant LD. Current issues in human lead exposure and regulation of lead. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:15–28. [PubMed]
6. Galal-Gorchev H. Dietary intake, levels in food and estimated intake of lead, cadmium, and mercury. Food Addit Contam. 1993;10:115–128. [PubMed]
7. Brody DJ, Pirkle JL, Kramer RA, et al. Blood lead levels in the US population. Phase 1 of the third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III, 1988-1991). JAMA. 1994;272:277–284. [PubMed]
8. Gellert GA, Wagner GA, Maxwell RM, et al. Lead poisoning among low-income children in Orange County, California. A need for regionally differentiated policy. JAMA. 1993;270:69–71. [PubMed]
9. Blumenthal HT, Mayfield R. An update on blood lead levels in pediatric patients of a neighborhood health center and an analysis of sources of exposure. J Natl Med Assoc. 1995;87:99–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
10. Rifai N, Cohen G, Wolf M, et al. Incidence of lead poisoning in young children from inner-city, suburban, and rural communities. Ther Drug Monit. 1993;15:71–74. [PubMed]
11. Gottlieb K, Koehler JR. Blood lead levels in children from lower socioeconomic communities in Denver, Colorado. Arch Environ Health. 1994;49:260–266. [PubMed]
12. Norman EH, Bordley WC, Hertz-Picciotto I, et al. Rural-urban blood lead differences in North Carolina children. Pediatrics. 1994;94:59–64. [PubMed]
13. Tejeda D, Wyatt D, Rostek B, et al. Do questions about lead exposure predict elevated lead levels? Pediatrics. 1994;93:192–194. [PubMed]
14. Binns HJ, LeBailly SA, Poncher J, et al. Is there lead in the suburbs? Risk assessment in Chicago suburban pediatric practices. Pediatrics. 1994;93:164–171. [PubMed]
15. Nordin JD, Rolnick SJ, Griffin JM. Prevalence of excess lead absorption and associated risk factors in children enrolled in a midwestern health maintenance organization. Pediatrics. 1994;93:172–177. [PubMed]
16. Taubman B, Wiley C, Henretig F. Prevalence of elevated blood lead levels in a suburban middle class private practice. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1994;148:757–760. [PubMed]
17. Maskarinec G. Blood lead levels among children in Hawaii. Hawaii Med J. 1993;52:242–250. [PubMed]
18. Cook M, Chappell WR, Hoffman RE, et al. Assessment of blood lead levels in children living in a historic mining and smelting community. Am J Epidemiol. 1993;137:447–455. [PubMed]
19. Kimbrough R, LeVois M, Webb D. Survey of lead exposure around a closed lead smelter. Pediatrics. 1995;95:550–554. [PubMed]
20. Fredeen DJ, Ehlinger EP, Cruikshank SH, et al. Lead levels among pregnant women in Hennepin County. Minn Med. 1992;75:29–32. [PubMed]
21. Flanigan GD. Jr, Mayfield R, Blumenthal HT. Studies on lead exposure in patients of a neighborhood health center: Part II. A comparison of women of childbearing age and children. J Natl Med Assoc. 1992;84:23–27. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
22. Sargent JD, Brown MJ, Freeman JL, et al. Childhood lead poisoning in Massachusetts communities: its association with sociodemographic and housing characteristics. Am J Public Health. 1995;85:528–534. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
23. Committee on Measuring Lead in Critical Populations, National Research Council. Measuring lead exposure in infants, children, and other sensitive populations. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. 1993
24. Agency for Toxic Substances and Disease Registry. The nature and extent of lead poisoning in children in the United States: a report to Congress. Atlanta: Department of Health and Human Services, Public Health Service. 1988;(Publication no. DHHS-99-2966.)
25. Department of Housing and Urban Development. Comprehensive and workable plan for the abatement of lead-based paint in privately owned housing. Report to Congress. Washington, DC: Department of Housing and Urban Development. 1990;(Publication no. HUD-PDR-1295.)
26. Environmental Protection Agency. Reducing lead in drinking water: a benefit analysis. Washington, DC: Environmental Protection Agency, Office of Policy Planning and Evaluation, 1986.
27. Arnetz BB, Nicolich MJ. Modelling of environmental lead contributors to blood lead in humans. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1990;62:397–402. [PubMed]
28. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Lead poisoning associated with use of traditional ethnic remedies—California, 1991-1992. MMWR. 1993;42:521–524. [PubMed]
29. Baghurst PA, McMichael AJ, Wigg NR, et al. Environmental exposure to lead and children's intelligence at the age of seven years. The Port Pirie Cohort Study. N Engl J Med. 1992;327:1279–1284. [PubMed]
30. Cooney G, Bell A, Stravou C. Low level exposure to lead and neurobehavioural development: the Sydney Study at seven yearsIn: Farmer JG, ed. Proceedings of Heavy Metals in the Environment Conference, Edinburgh. September 1991:116–19.
31. Bellinger DC, Stiles KM, Needleman HL. Low-level lead exposure, intelligence and academic achievement: a long-term follow-up study. Pediatrics. 1992;90:855–861. [PubMed]
32. Dietrich KN, Berger OG, Succop PA, et al. The developmental consequences of low to moderate prenatal and postnatal lead exposure: intellectual attainment in the Cincinnati Lead Study Cohort following school entry. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1993;15:37–44. [PubMed]
33. Ernhart CB, Morrow-Tlucak M, Wolf AW, et al. Low-level lead exposure in the prenatal and early preschool periods: intelligence prior to school entry. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1989;11:161–170. [PubMed]
34. Wasserman GA, Graziano JH, Factor-Litvak P, et al. Consequences of lead exposure and iron supplementation on childhood development at age 4 years. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1994;16:233–240. [PubMed]
35. Wolf AW, Jimenez E, Lozoff B. No evidence of developmental III effects of low-level lead exposure in a developing country. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1994;15:224–231. [PubMed]
36. Pocock SJ, Smith M, Baghurst P. Environmental lead and children's intelligence: a systematic review of the epidemiological evidence. BMJ. 1994;309:1189–1197. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
37. Needleman HL, Gatsonis CA. Low-level lead exposure and the IQ of children. A meta-analysis of modern studies. JAMA. 1990;263:673–678. [PubMed]
38. Winneke G, Brockhaus A, Ewers U, et al. Results from the European multicenter study on lead neurotoxicity in children: implications for risk assessment. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1990;12:553–559. [PubMed]
39. Winneke G, Beginn U, Ewert T, et al. Comparing the effects of perinatal and later childhood lead exposure on neuropsychological outcome. Environ Res. 1985;38:155–167. [PubMed]
40. Winneke G, Kramer U, Brockhaus A, et al. Neuropsychological studies in children with elevated tooth-lead concentrations. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1983;51:231–252. [PubMed]
41. Winneke G, Brockhaus A, Collet W, et al. Modulation of lead-induced performance deficit in children by varying signal rate in a serial choice reaction task. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1989;11:587–592. [PubMed]
42. Hatzakis A, Kokkevi A, Katsouyanni K, et al. Psychometric intelligence and attentional performance deficits in lead-exposed children. In: Proceedings of the International Conference: heavy metals in the environment. 1987:1204–209.
43. Otto DA, Fox DA. Auditory and visual dysfunction following lead exposure. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:191–208. [PubMed]
44. Cory-Slechta DA. The behavioral toxicity of lead: problems and perspectives. Adv Behav Pharmacol. 1984;4:211–255.
45. Rice DC. Lead-induced changes in learning: evidence for behavioral mechanisms from experimental animal studies. Neurotoxicol. 1993;14:167–178. [PubMed]
46. Staessen JA, Lauwerys RR, Buchet J-P, et al. Impairment of renal function with increasing blood lead concentrations in the general population. N Engl J Med. 1992;327:151–156. [PubMed]
47. Factor-Litvak P, Stein Z, Graziano J. Increased risk of proteinuria among a cohort of lead-exposed pregnant women. Environ Health Perspect. 1993;101:418–421. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
48. Graziano JH, Slavkovic V, Factor-Litvak P, et al. Depressed serum erythropoietin in pregnant women with elevated blood lead. Arch Environ Health. 1991;46:347–350. [PubMed]
49. Ernhart CB, Wolf AW, Kennard MJ, et al. Intrauterine exposure to low levels of lead: the status of the neonate. Arch Environ Health. 1986;41:287–291. [PubMed]
50. McMichael AJ, Vimpani GV, Robertson EF, et al. The Port Pirie cohort study: maternal blood lead and pregnancy outcome. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1986;40:18–25. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
51. Bellinger D, Leviton A, Rabinowitz M, et al. Weight gain and maturity in fetuses exposed to low levels of lead. Environ Res. 1991;54:151–158. [PubMed]
52. Murphy MJ, Graziano JH, Popovac D, et al. Past pregnancy outcomes among women living in the vicinity of a lead smelter in Kosovo, Yugoslavia. Am J Public Health. 1990;80:33–35. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
53. Lindbohm ML, Sallmen M, Anttila A, et al. Paternal occupational lead exposure and spontaneous abortion. Scand J Work Environ Health. 1991;17:95–103. [PubMed]
54. Laudanski T, Sipowicz M, Modzelewski P, et al. Influence of high lead and cadmium soil content on human reproductive outcome. Int J Gynecol Obstet. 1991;36:309–315. [PubMed]
55. Dietrich KN, Krafft KM, Bornschein RL, et al. Low-level fetal lead exposure effect on neurobehavioral development in early infancy. Pediatrics. 1987;80:721–730. [PubMed]
56. Factor-Litvak P, Graziano JH, Kline JK, et al. A prospective study of birthweight and length of gestation in a population surrounding a lead smelter in Kosovo, Yugoslavia. Int J Epidemiol. 1991;20:722–728. [PubMed]
57. Milman N, Christensen JM, Ibsen KK. Blood lead and erythrocyte zinc protoporphyrin in mothers and newborn infants. Eur J Pediatr. 1988;147:71–73. [PubMed]
58. Ernhart CB. A critical review of low-level prenatal lead exposure in the human: I. Effects on the fetus and newborn. Reprod Toxicol. 1992;6:9–19. [PubMed]
59. Baghurst PA, Robertson EF, Oldfield RK, et al. Lead in the placenta, membranes, and umbilical cord in relation to pregnancy outcome in a lead-smelter community. Environ Health Perspect. 1991;90:315–320. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
60. Chisolm JJ Jr. The use of chelating agents in the treatment of acute and chronic lead intoxication in childhood. J Pediatr. 1968;73:1–38. [PubMed]
61. Pueschel SM, Kopito L, Schwachman H. Children with an increased lead burden: a screening and follow-up study. JAMA. 1972;222:462–466. [PubMed]
62. Nuyts GD, Daelemans RA, Jorens PG, et al. Does lead play a role in the development of chronic renal disease? Nephrol Dial Transplant. 1991;6:307–315. [PubMed]
63. Hu H. A 50-year follow-up of childhood plumbism. Hypertension, renal function, and hemoglobin levels among survivors. Am J Dis Child. 1991;145:681–687. [PubMed]
64. Grandjean P, Hollnagel H, Hedegaard L, et al. Blood lead-blood pressure relations: alcohol intake and hemoglobin as confounders. Am J Epidemiol. 1989;129:732–739. [PubMed]
65. Elwood PC, Yarnell JWG, Oldham PD, et al. Blood pressure and blood lead in surveys in Wales. Am J Epidemiol. 1988;127:942–945. [PubMed]
66. Schwartz J. Lead, blood pressure, and cardiovascular disease in men and women. Environ Health Perspect. 1991;91:71–75. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
67. Moller L, Kristensen TS. Blood lead as a cardiovascular risk factor. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;136:1091–1100. [PubMed]
68. Dolenc P, Staessen JA, Lauwerys RR, et al. Short report: low-level lead exposure does not increase the blood pressure in the general population. J Hypertens. 1993;11:589–593. [PubMed]
69. Hense HW, Filipiak B, Keil U. The association of blood lead and blood pressure in population surveys. Epidemiology. 1993;4:173–179. [PubMed]
70. Rabinowitz M, Bellinger D, Leviton A, et al. Pregnancy hypertension, blood pressure during labor, and blood lead levels. Hypertension. 1987;10:447–451. [PubMed]
71. Selbst SM, Sokas RK, Henretig FM, et al. The effect of blood lead on blood pressure in children. J Environ Pathol Toxicol Oncol. 1993;12:213–218. [PubMed]
72. Schwartz J, Angle C, Pitcher H. Relationship between childhood blood lead levels and stature. Pediatrics. 1986;77:281–288. [PubMed]
73. Frisancho AR, Ryan AS. Decreased stature associated with moderate blood lead concentrations in Mexican-American children. Am J Clin Nutr. 1991;54:516–519. [PubMed]
74. Shukla R, Dietrich KN, Bornschein RL, et al. Lead exposure and growth in the early preschool child: a follow-up report from the Cincinnati Lead Study. Pediatrics. 1991;88:886–892. [PubMed]
75. Greene T, Ernhart CB. Prenatal and preschool age lead exposure: relationship with size. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1991;13:417–427. [PubMed]
76. Jacobson BE, Lockitch G, Quigley G. Improved sample preparation for accurate determination of low concentrations of lead in whole blood by graphite furnace analysis. Clin Chem. 1991;37:515–519. [PubMed]
77. Parsons PJ, Raciti KA, Esernio-Jenssen D. Evaluation and improvement of sample collection procedures for the determination of blood lead. Third semi-annual report to the Center for Environmental Health and Injury Control, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 1993.
78. Schlenker TL, Fritz CJ, Mark D, et al. Screening for pediatric lead poisoning: comparability of simultaneously drawn capillary and venous blood samples. JAMA. 1994;271:1346–1348. [PubMed]
79. Schonfeld DJ, Cullen MR, Rainey PM, et al. Screening for lead poisoning in an urban pediatric clinic using samples obtained by fingerstick. Pediatrics. 1994;94:174–179. [PubMed]
80. Lyngbye T, Jorgensen PJ, Grandjean P, et al. Validity and interpretation of blood lead levels: a study of Danish school children. Scand J Clin Lab Invest. 1990;50:441–449. [PubMed]
81. Schonfeld DJ, Rainey PM, Cullen MR, et al. Screening for lead poisoning by fingerstick in suburban pediatric practices. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 1995;149:447–450. [PubMed]
82. Parsons PJ. Monitoring human exposure to lead: an assessment of current laboratory performance for the determination of blood lead. Environ Res. 1992;57:149–162. [PubMed]
83. Blumenthal HT, Flanigan GD, Mayfield R. Studies on lead exposure in patients of a neighborhood health center: Part I. Pediatric patients. J Natl Med Assoc. 1991;83:1065–1072. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
84. Mahaffey KR, Annest JL. Association of erythrocyte protoporphyrin with blood lead level and iron status in the second National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1979-1980. Environ Res. 1986;41:328–338. [PubMed]
85. Debaun MR, Sox HC. Setting the optimal erythrocyte protoporphyrin screening decision threshold for lead poisoning: a decision analytic approach. Pediatrics. 1991;88:121–131. [PubMed]
86. Parsons PJ, Reilly AA, Hussain A. Observational study of erythrocyte protoporphyrin screening test for detecting low lead exposure in children: impact of lowering the blood lead action threshold. Clin Chem. 1991;37:216–225. [PubMed]
87. McElvaine MD, Orbach HG, Binder S, et al. Evaluation of the erythrocyte protoporphyrin test as a screen for elevated blood lead levels. J Pediatr. 1991;119:548–550. [PubMed]
88. Leung FY, Bradley C, Pellar TG. Reference intervals for blood lead and evaluation of zinc protoporphyrin as a screening test for lead toxicity. Clin Biochem. 1993;26:491–496. [PubMed]
89. Rolfe PB, Marcinak JF, Nice AJ, et al. Use of zinc protoporphyrin measured by the Protofluor-Z hematofluorometer in screening children for elevated blood lead level. Am J Dis Child. 1993;147:66–68. [PubMed]
90. Marcus AH, Schwartz J. Dose-response curves for erythrocyte protoporphyrin vs blood lead: effects of iron status. Environ Res. 1987;44:221–227. [PubMed]
91. Guthrie R, Orfanos A, Widger K, et al. Screening suburban/rural children for lead exposure, iron deficiency. Am J Public Health. 1988;78:856–857. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
92. Schaffer SJ, Szilagyi PG, Weitzman M. Lead poisoning risk determination in an urban population through the use of a standardized questionnaire. Pediatrics. 1994;93:159–163. [PubMed]
93. Rooney BL, Hayes EB, Allen BK, et al. Development of a screening tool for prediction of children at risk for lead exposure in a midwestern clinical setting. Pediatrics. 1994;93:183–187. [PubMed]
93A. Striph KB. Prevalence of lead poisoning in a suburban practice. J Fam Pract. 1995;41:65–71. [PubMed]
94. McCusker J. Longitudinal changes in blood lead level in children and their relationship to season, age, and exposure to paint or plaster. Am J Public Health. 1979;69:348–352. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
95. Reigart JR, Whitlock NH. Longitudinal observations of the relationship between free erythrocyte porphyrins and whole blood lead. Pediatrics. 1976;57:54–59. [PubMed]
96. Sachs HK, Blanksma LA, Murray EF, et al. Ambulatory treatment of lead poisoning: report of 1,155 cases. Pediatrics. 1970;46:389–396. [PubMed]
97. Sachs HK. Effect of a screening program on changing patterns of lead poisoning. Environ Health Perspect. 1974;7:41–45. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
98. Browder A, Joselow M, Louria DB, et al. Evaluation of screening programs for childhood lead poisoning by analysis of hospital admissions. Am J Public Health. 1974;64:914–915. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
99. Ruff HA, Bijur PE, Markowitz M, et al. Declining blood lead levels and cognitive changes in moderately lead-poisoned children [comments in JAMA. 1993;270:827–829]. [PubMed]
100. Markowitz ME, Bijur PE, Ruff H, et al. Effects of calcium disodium versenate (CaNa2EDTA) chelation in moderate childhood lead poisoning. Pediatrics. 1993;92:265–271. [PubMed]
101. Moel DI, Sachs HK, Drayton MA. Slow, natural reduction in blood lead level after chelation therapy for lead poisoning in childhood. Am J Dis Child. 1986;140:905–908. [PubMed]
102. Chisolm JJ Jr. Chelation therapy in children with subclinical plumbism. Pediatrics. 1974;53:441–443. [PubMed]
103. Vitale LF, Rosalinas-Bailon A, Folland D, et al. Oral penicillamine therapy for chronic lead poisoning in children. J Pediatr. 1973;83:1041–1045. [PubMed]
104. Graziano JH, LoIacono Lolacono NJ, Moulton T, et al. Controlled study of meso-2,3-dimercaptosuccinic acid for the management of childhood lead intoxication. J Pediatr. 1992;120:133–139. [PubMed]
105. Marcus SM. Experience with D-penicillamine in treating lead poisoning. Vet Hum Toxicol. 1982;24:18–20. [PubMed]
106. Graziano JH, LoIacono Lolacono NJ, Meyer P. Dose-response study of oral 2,3-dimercaptosuccinic acid in children with elevated blood lead concentrations. J Pediatr. 1988;113:751–757. [PubMed]
107. Shannon M, Graef J, Lovejoy FH Jr. Efficacy and toxicity of D-penicillamine in low-level lead poisoning. J Pediatr. 1988;112:799–804. [PubMed]
108. Liebelt EL, Shannon M, Graef JW. Efficacy of oral meso-2,3-dimercaptosuccinic acid therapy for low-level childhood plumbism. J Pediatr. 1994;124:313–317. [PubMed]
109. Shannon M, Grace A, Graef JW. Use of penicillamine in children with small lead burdens [letter] N Engl J Med. 1989;321:979–980. [PubMed]
110. Office of Policy Development and Research, Department of Housing and Urban Development. Lead-based paint abatement demonstration (FHA). Washington, DC: Department of Housing and Urban Development. 1991;(Publication no. HUD-1316-PDR.)
111. Farfel MR, Chisolm JJ Jr, Rohde CA. The longer-term effectiveness of residential lead paint abatement. Environ Res. 1994;66:217–221. [PubMed]
112. Chisolm JJ Jr, Millits ED, Quaskey SA. The relationship between the level of lead absorption in children and the age, type and condition of housing. Environ Res. 1985;38:31–45. [PubMed]
113. Farfel MR, Chisolm JJ Jr. Health and environmental outcomes of traditional and modified practices for abatement of residential lead-based paint. Am J Public Health. 1990;80:1240–1245. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
114. Staes C, Matte T, Copley CG, et al. Retrospective study of the impact of lead-based paint hazard remediation on children's blood lead levels in St. Louis, Missouri. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;139:1016–1026. [PubMed]
115. Rust SW, Burgoon DA, Pierce BK, et al. Draft report on a summary of studies addressing the efficacy of lead abatement. Columbus, OH: Battelle. 1993
116. Amitai Y, Brown MJ, Graef JW, et al. Residential deleading: effects on the blood lead levels of lead-poisoned children. Pediatrics. 1991;88:893–897. [PubMed]
117. Rosen JF, Markowitz ME, Bijur PE, et al. L-line x-ray fluorescence of cortical bone lead compared with the CaNa2EDTA test in lead-toxic children [Correction published in Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1989;86:7595] Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1989;86:685–689. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
118. Rosen JF, Markowitz ME, Bijur PE, et al. Sequential measurements of bone lead content by L x-ray fluorescence in CaNa2EDTA-treated lead-toxic children. Environ Health Perspect. 1991;93:271–277. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
119. Rosen JF, Markowitz ME. Trends in the management of childhood lead poisonings. Neurotoxicology. 1993;14:211–218. [PubMed]
120. Swindell S, Charney E. Does home abatement lower blood lead levels in children with Class II lead poisoning [abstr]? Am J Dis Child. 1992; 1994;146
121. Mushak P, Crocetti AF. Methods for reducing lead exposure in young children and other risk groups: an integrated summary of a report to the U.S. Congress on childhood lead poisoning. Environ Health Perspect. 1990;89:125–135. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
122. Foster JD, Louria DB, Stinson L. Influence of documented lead poisoning on environmental modification programs in Newark, New Jersey. Arch Environ Health. 1979;34:368–371. [PubMed]
123. Weitzman M, Aschengrau A, Bellinger D, et al. Lead-contaminated soil abatement and urban children's blood lead levels [comments in JAMA 1993; 270: 829-830] JAMA. 1993;269:1647–1654. [PubMed]
124. Environmental Protection Agency. Three city urban soil lead abatement demonstration project (fact sheet). Washington, DC: Environmental Protection Agency. 1992;(Publication no. 9355.4-10FSa.)
125. Charney E, Kessler B, Farfel M, et al. Childhood lead poisoning. A controlled trial of the effect of dust-control measures on blood lead levels. N Engl J Med. 1983;309:1089–1093. [PubMed]
126. Kimbrough RD, LeVois M, Webb DR. Management of children with slightly elevated blood lead levels. Pediatrics. 1994;93:188–191. [PubMed]
127. Porru S, Donato F, Apostoli P, et al. The utility of health education among lead workers: the experience of one program. Am J Ind Med. 1993;22:473–481. [PubMed]
127A. Kimbrough R, LeVois M, Webb D. Survey of lead exposure around a closed lead smelter. Pediatrics. 1995;95:550–554. [PubMed]
128. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Preventing lead poisoning in young children. A statement by the Centers for Disease Control—October 1991. Atlanta: Department of Health and Human Services. 1991
129. Mahaffey KR. Nutritional factors in lead poisoning. Nutr Rev. 1981;39:353–362. [PubMed]]
130. Ziegler EE, Edwards BB, Jensen RL, et al. Absorption and retention of lead by infants. Pediatr Res. 1978;12:29–34. [PubMed]
131. Markowitz ME, Rosen JF, Bijur PE. Effects of iron deficiency on lead excretion in children with moderate lead intoxication. J Pediatr. 1990;116:360–364. [PubMed]
132. Ernhart CB, Wolf AW, Sokol RJ, et al. Fetal lead exposure: antenatal factors. Environ Res. 1985;38:54–66. [PubMed]
133. Carraccio CL, Bergman GE, Daley BP. Combined iron deficiency and lead poisoning in children. Clin Pediatr. 1987;26:644–647. [PubMed]
134. Graziano JH, Popovac D, Factor-Litvak, et al. Determinants of elevated blood lead during pregnancy in a population surrounding a lead smelter in Kosovo, Yugoslavia. Environ Health Perspect. 1990;89:95–100. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
135. Glotzer DE, Bauchner H. Management of childhood lead poisoning: a survey. Pediatrics. 1992;89:614–618. [PubMed]
136. Markowitz ME, Rosen JF. Need for the lead mobilization test in children with lead poisoning. J Pediatr. 1991;119:305–310. [PubMed]
137. Amitai Y, Graef JW, Brown MJ, et al. Hazards of "deleading" homes of children with lead poisoning. Am J Dis Child. 1987;141:758–760. [PubMed]
138. Rey-Alvarez S, Menke-Hargrave T. Deleading dilemma: pitfall in the management of childhood lead poisoning. Pediatrics. 1987;79:214–217. [PubMed]
139. Feldman RG. Urban lead mining: lead intoxication among deleaders. N Engl J Med. 1978;298:1143–1145. [PubMed]
140. Chisolm JJ Jr. Treatment of acute lead intoxication—choice of chelating agents and supportive therapeutic measures. Clin Toxicol. 1970;3:527–540. [PubMed]
141. Moel DI, Kumar K. Reversible nephrotoxic reactions to a combined 2,3-dimercapto-1-propanol and calcium disodium ethylenediaminetetraacetic acid regimen in asymptomatic children with elevated blood lead levels. Pediatrics. 1982;70:259–262. [PubMed]
142. Mann KV, Travers JD. Succimer, an oral lead chelator. Clin Pharmacol. 1991(10):914–922. [PubMed]
143. Alliance to End Childhood Lead Poisoning. Guide to state lead screening laws. Washington, DC: Alliance to End Childhood Lead Poisoning, 1991.
144. Medicaid Bureau, Health Care Financing Administration, Department of Health and Human Services. EPSDT: a guide for educational programs. Baltimore: Department of Health and Human Services, 1992.
145. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Environmental Health. Lead poisoning: from screening to primary prevention. Pediatrics. 1993;92:176–183. [PubMed]
146. Green M, ed. Arlington, VA; 1994. Bright Futures: guidelines for health supervision of infants, children and adolescents.
147. American Academy of Family Physicians. Kansas City, MO; 1994. Age charts for periodic health examination. (Reprint no. 510.)
148. Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination. Canadian guide to clinical preventive health care. Ottawa: Canada Communication Group. 1994:268–288.
149. American Medical Association Council on Scientific Affairs. Report 6: lead poisoning among children. Chicago: American Medical Association, December. 1994
150. Prpic-Majic D, Pongracic J, Hrsak J, et al. A follow-up study in a lead smelter community following the introduction of an effective pollution control system. Isr J Med Sci. 1992;28:548–556. [PubMed]
151. Yankel AJ, von Lindern IH, Walter SD. The Silver Valley Study: the relationship between childhood blood lead levels and environmental exposure. J Air Pollut Control Assoc. 1977;27:763–767. [PubMed]
152. Department of Health and Human Services, Department of Housing and Urban Development, and Environmental Protection Agency. Report of the Interagency Task Force on the Prevention of Lead Poisoning: a report to Congress. Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services, December 1993.

Return to Table of Contents